The Brooding Mind
Imagine yourself at your 10-year high school reunion, a long
anticipated get-together for you and all your old friends. You haven’t seen
many of them since graduation day, and naturally everyone is comparing notes on
the lives they have lived since then. This puts you in a reflective mood, but
not in a good way. Life has been unkind to you—compared to the lives of your
friends, who have all been spared your travails. For days after the reunion,
you can’t focus on anything but your difficulties, and the unfairness of it
all.
If you’re a brooder, that is. Someone else might have the
same reunion experience, yet come away with a very different interpretation.
Every life has its ups and downs, and yours is not unusually good or bad.
That’s life.
Brooding is a particularly toxic kind of rumination, and
it’s strongly associated with clinical depression. Brooders see their own
problems as debilitating, and this self-focus sabotages any real effort to make
things better. It leads to all sorts of negative feelings, which in turn lead
to more ruminative thinking, creating a perilous cycle of thought and emotion.
But what’s the origin of such brooding? Psychological
scientists have in recent years been examining cognitive bias as a contributor
to mental disorders, including depression. Many events in our lives are
ambiguous, and we all have this basic urge to resolve life’s ambiguities. But
while most of us interpret such ambiguous experiences in a neutral or benign
way, others are powerfully biased toward the negative. This bias in
interpretation could play an important causal role in brooding and depression.
That’s the idea that psychological scientist Paula Hertel,
of Trinity University in San Antonio, has been exploring in her laboratory.
Working with Nilly Mor of Hebrew University and other Trinity colleagues,
Hertel designed two experiments to target negative interpretations of ambiguity
as a possible cause of brooding—and depression.
For the first study, the scientists created 40 scenarios
like the high school reunion described above—ambiguous scenarios that could be
seen in different ways. These scenarios were conducive to negative thinking
about oneself, but they could also be interpreted in benign fashion. Half of
the scenarios ended with a word fragment that pointed to a ruminative
interpretation, while the other half ended in a way that did not. The scientists
measured the speed with which participants resolved each type of fragment—as an
indicator of distorted thinking.
The participants themselves had either very high brooding
scores, or very low, on a standard measure of this trait. Of course, not even
extreme brooders ruminate in every situation they encounter, so the scientists
primed some to focus on themselves, while distracting the others from
self-focus. They were then told to imagine themselves in these various
situations—actually rubbing shoulders with old high school classmates, for
example. The scientists predicted that those with brooding tendencies would be
quick to resolves scenarios negatively, especially if they were already focused
on themselves.
And that’s just what they found. Brooders who were focused
on themselves to begin with—these subjects were clearly biased toward
negativity. The non-brooders showed no such bias, nor did any of those who were
distracted. These findings point to a negativity bias in brooders’
interpretations of life’s ambiguity.
The scientists wanted to double-check this intriguing
finding, in a different way. So in a second study, they tried to simulate the
cognitive bias in order to see if it contributed to rumination. They recruited
volunteers—in this case all non-brooders—and “trained” them to make either
negative or benign interpretations of ambiguous situations like the reunion.
They wanted to see if this repeated practice in biased thinking would establish
a habit of mind—one that would transfer to other situations.
And again, the answer is yes. As reported in an article to
appear in the journal Clinical Psychological Science, subjects who were trained
in negative interpretations—these subjects subsequently revealed that bias in
their writing. What’s more, this cognitive bias affected the participants’
memories. This makes sense, since recall is shaped, as a “side effect,” by
interpretation of events: “Once interpreted, thus remembered,” Hertel says.
These lab results have some real life implications, and
indeed offer hope. If a negativity bias does indeed to lead rumination, and is
malleable, how about doing the opposite? That is, perhaps habitual ruminators
could benefit from some “bright side” training, learning to make benign or even
positive interpretations of ambiguous experiences. Such interventions might
diminish this maladaptive habit of mind.
The habit of brooding does not develop overnight, and will
not be broken easily. Such change, Hertel and the others say, may require
intense training in cognitive control, of the sort offered through mindfulness
training. But the current insight could prove a valuable first step.
See you all tomorrow.
Buh-bye.
But at the close of the day, Nuclear Blast made us a good offer and thus it was time to go ahead. Learn more about One Pearl Bank Location on this site.
ReplyDeleteFirst year students are always assigned to the standard halls ahead of their arrival. If you are curious to know more about avenue south residence location, click here.
ReplyDeleteRemember that the broker is the one who will be getting paid if the lease is signed so it is important that you get the most for your money. Get more details about commercial property for lease visit here.
ReplyDeleteRemember that the broker is the one who will be getting paid if the lease is signed so it is important that you get the most for your money. Get more details about commercial property for lease visit here.
ReplyDelete