You Have A Hive Mind
Every decision you make is essentially a committee act.
Members chime in, options are weighed, and eventually a single proposal for
action is approved by consensus. The committee, of course, is the densely knit
society of neurons in your head. And “approved by consensus” is really just a
delicate way of saying that the opposition was silenced.
Our brains seem to work not by generating only “correct”
actions and executing them in serial, but rather by representing many
possibilities in parallel, and suppressing all but one. When this inhibitory
action is lost, as happens in people with frontal lobe damage, these multiple
possibilities become a burden, and can lead to so-called utilization behaviors.
Such impaired individuals will indiscriminately reach for objects placed in
front of them - a hairbrush or a hammer, for example - and use them even in
inappropriate contexts.
In essence, despite our feeling that we are singular,
unified agents, we are more like hive minds unto ourselves, our brains abuzz
with multiple, often conflicting plans and interests that must be managed. To
Dr. Thomas Seeley, a professor of neurobiology at Cornell University, the “hive
mind” is more than just a metaphor. In a recent paper in Science, Seeley and
his colleagues describe a potential deep parallel between how brains and bee
swarms come to a decision. With no central planner or decider, both brains and
bee hives can resolve their inner differences to commit to single courses of
action.
To watch a group of bees is to see a frenzy of different
interests coalesce into a single, clear thought. This is analogous to neurons
in the brain, which must reach a consensus on how to achieve a behavioral goal
by positioning the body in space. Bees in a hive must do something similar when
deciding where to move the superorganism that is the swarm. Failing to move the
swarm as a single, committed unit risks splitting up the hive and losing the
queen. Similarly, making a poor move could expose the hive to predators or
extreme temperatures.
Like many other decision-makers, the hive’s first order of
business before making a springtime move is to consider the various
possibilities. Toward this end, several groups of scouts are sent off to search
for a suitable new hive. When the scouts return, they each advocate for
preferred new sites - often different ones - by performing the famed “waggle
dance,” a figure-eight series of movements that tells other bees the direction
and distance to a potential new site. These dances recruit other uncommitted
bees in the hive to also advocate for the advertised site.
For a while, many scientists thought that this strategy of
steadily accumulating “votes” for a particular location was sufficient to
explain the hive’s eventual decision. Others, including Seeley and his
colleagues, were not satisfied. What happens in cases where similarly sized
groups of bees are advocating for different locations? Wouldn’t this be a
formula for deadlock?
Seeley suspected that the answer had to do with a
head-butting move bees make. To explore this idea, he and his team first set up
swarms on an island lacking natural nests, and gave scouts a choice between two
identical artificial nesting boxes. Scouts that visited one site were marked
with yellow paint, while scouts visiting the other site were marked with pink
paint. By tagging these two different populations, Seeley and colleagues had in
a sense labeled two competing ideas, which they could then watch unfold and
interact back in the collective hive mind.
The researchers found that the yellow and pink-painted
scouts displayed waggle dances advertising for their respective nests. In
addition, however, the scouts were also seen to make brief buzzing head-butts
to one another’s head and thorax. Dancing bees tended to receive head-butts
toward the end of their dances, suggesting that the head butts were a signal to
stop dancing. The most interesting finding came when looking at who was
head-butting whom. Yellow-marked bees tended to receive these putative stop
signals from pink-marked bees, and vice versa. In other words, the two
different populations were mutually inhibiting one another - one proposal
pitted against another.
The result of this arrangement is that it amplifies small
differences between different populations of scouts, setting up a kind of
winner-take-all scenario. Without inhibitory stop signals, the hive would be
able to sustain multiple competing interests, as different groups of scouts
accumulate more and more votes until the hive reaches some stable, but divided
state. With stop signals, divided hive states are far less stable. A slight
preponderance of one group of scouts will translate into greater inhibition of
other groups of scouts, turning an initially small numerical advantage into a
more sizable one. Over several iterations of this process, an initial slight
majority is amplified into a consensus.
Ideally, a follow-up experiment would have eliminated the
bees’ stop signals and studied the consequences on the hive’s decision process.
Since this is nearly impossible to do, Seeley and his colleagues opted for a
simulation based approach instead. In their models of collective bee activity,
cross-inhibitory stop signals were essential for breaking decision deadlocks
between two equally attractive nests. If the stop signals were indiscriminate,
or absent altogether, the hive remained split, and never converged on a
consensus.
Seeley and his team propose that cross-inhibition may be a
general strategy for decision making, and indeed, their findings in bees
recapitulate features of decision making and pattern formation in other
systems. The remarkable unifying theme in all of these systems is how an
aggregate swarm intelligence is built from just a few kinds of simple, local
interactions between agents. Both neurons and bees are presumably unaware of
how their impulses and signals transcend the individual, and lay the substrate
for a grander, collective intelligence.
See you all tomorrow.
Buh-bye.
Comments
Post a Comment